geometric hallucination

geometric hallucination
   Also known as geometrical hallucination, geometric visual hallucination, and optogeometric illusion. All four terms can be traced to the Greek noun geometria, which means land surveying. They are used to denote a * formed visual hallucination characterized by lines, planes, * spirals, and/or other geometrical forms or patterns. Some examples of geometric hallucinations are circles, ellipses, * herringbone patterns, spider webs, * lattices, * gratings, kaleidoscopes, mandalas, mosaics, symmetrical flowerlike patterns, and *fortifications. When they consist of irregular branching forms they are referred to as * dendropsia. When they consist of two-dimensional geometric forms such as tiles, squares, triangles, or hexagonal forms, they are referred to as tessellated hallucinations or *tessellopsia. Attempts to arrange geometric hallucinations in accordance with their phenomenological characteristics have yielded a classification of *form-constants devised by the German-American biological psychologist and philosopher Heinrich Klüver (1897-1979), and a classification of * dimensions of visual imagery devised by the American psychopharmacologists Ronald K. Siegel and Murray E. Jarvik. As to the pathophysiological correlates of geometric hallucinations, early hypotheses tended to focus on the involvement of the retina's choriocapillary circulation or its regularly arranged rods and cones, which were thought to be rendered visible by means of a process called 'transient retroreti-nal illumination'. Today, however, this entoptic model accounts for the mediation of no more than a fraction of the geometric hallucinations. As already noted by Klüver, geometric hallucinations tend to change places in keeping with the movements of the eyes, but to maintain their position relative to each other, suggesting that they are not mediated by the eye itself, but by the brain. As a consequence, the entoptic model of geometric hallucinations has now been largely abandoned in favour of a central model which attributes the mediation ofthe geometric patterns to neuronal discharges affecting the retinocorti-cal map (i.e. the patterns of connection between the retina and striate cortex), and/or neuronal circuits lying within striate cortex. Etiologically, geometric hallucinations are associated with a variety of neurological disorders (notably * aurae occurring in the context of paroxysmal neurological disorders such as migraine and epilepsy), *psychosis, and intoxication with substances such as cannabis, LSD, mescaline, alkaloids, antihis-tamines, nicotine, amphetamines, carbon dioxide, nitrous oxide, and numerous therapeutics and anaesthetics. They also occur regularly in the context of * hypnagogic and (to a lesser extent) *hypnopompic imagery. Moreover, some of the phenomena in the category geometric hallucination can be evoked experimentally by exerting bilateral pressure on the eye bulbs (resulting in so-called * pressure phosphenes), through the use of a flickering light (resulting in *photically induced hallucinations) or a weak electrical current applied to the eyeball or the visual cortex (as in cortical probing), and with the aid of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of the visual cortex. It has been suggested that geometric hallucinations share certain mechanisms for the activation of visual representation with those involved in the mediation of * mosaic vision. The term geometric hallucination derives from a classification of hallucinations governed by the theme of complexity. It is used in opposition to the terms * simple (or * elementary) hallucination and *complex hallucination.
   References
   Bressloff, P.C., Cowan, J.D., Golubitsky, M., Thomas, P.J., Wiener, M.C. (2001). Geometric visual hallucinations, Euclidian symmetry and the functional architecture of striate cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 356, 299-330.
   Klüver, H. (1966). Mescal and Mechanisms of hallucinations. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
   Marshall, C.R. (1937). An inquiry into the cause of mescal visions. Journal of Neurology and Psychopathology, 17, 289-304.
   Siegel, R.K., Jarvik, M.E. (1975). Drug-induced hallucinations in animals and man.In: Hallucinations. Behavior, experience, and theory. Edited by Siegel, R.K., West, L.J. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.

Dictionary of Hallucinations. . 2010.

Игры ⚽ Поможем написать реферат

Look at other dictionaries:

  • geometric visual hallucination —    see geometric hallucination …   Dictionary of Hallucinations

  • Hallucination — For other uses, see Hallucination (disambiguation). Hallucination Classification and external resources My eyes at the moment of the apparitions by August Natterer …   Wikipedia

  • complex hallucination —    A term used to denote a phenomenologically rich and often well organized type of hallucination that is confined to a single sensory modality. Theoretically, complex hallucinations may present in any of the sensory modalities. It is customary,… …   Dictionary of Hallucinations

  • geometrical hallucination —    see geometric hallucination …   Dictionary of Hallucinations

  • photically induced hallucination —    Also known as flicker induced hallucination, hallucinatory flicker induced experiences, and Purkinje s colours. All four terms are used to denote a type of *geometric visual hallucination induced by a flickering light, such as that produced by …   Dictionary of Hallucinations

  • formed hallucination —    Also known as formed visual hallucination, formed vision, organized hallucination, and morphopsia. All five terms are used to denote a visual hallucination depicting a distinctive shape, pattern, object, or scene. Thus the category of formed… …   Dictionary of Hallucinations

  • primitive hallucination —    A term used (and possibly also introduced) in 1930 by the German psychologists Konrad Zucker and Julius Zâdor in the context of a study of * mescaline induced hallucinations to denote a relatively simple type of hallucination. Judging by the… …   Dictionary of Hallucinations

  • closed-eye hallucination —    Also known as closed eye visual hallucination and closed eye visualization. All three terms are used to denote a type of *visual hallucination experienced exclusively when the eyes are closed or when there is perfect darkness around. Some… …   Dictionary of Hallucinations

  • unimodal hallucination —    The term unimodal hallucination is indebted to the Latin words unus (one) and modus (means, manner). It is used to denote a hallucination that confines itself to a single sensory modality. Unimodal hallucinations can be either * elementary, *… …   Dictionary of Hallucinations

  • visual hallucination —    Also referred to as vision. Both terms are indebted to the Latin noun visio, which means sight. They are used to denote a hallucination of sight. Historically, visual hallucinations have been divided into a multitude of types. Using their… …   Dictionary of Hallucinations

Share the article and excerpts

Direct link
Do a right-click on the link above
and select “Copy Link”